Discussion 1: Attitude Formation

Discussion 1: Attitude Formation

Discussion 1: Attitude Formation

Advertisers successfully use classical conditioning strategies to persuade consumers. Imagine that you are an advertising manager. For this Discussion, imagine the following scenario:

ORDER NOW FOR COMPREHENSIVE, PLAGIARISM-FREE PAPERS

You are the advertising manager at an agency that has been hired by a corporation preparing to launch a new product. Your assignment is to design an advertisement that promises to popularize the new product and ultimately boost the corporation’s profit margin. A national ad launch guarantees the new product will have wide visibility, putting pressure on you to deliver a message that will encourage consumers to purchase the product.

For this Discussion, you will apply classical conditioning strategies to an ad campaign and analyze the formation of attitudes by classical conditioning. Discussion 1: Attitude Formation

 

To Prepare

  • Review the Learning Resources for this week and consider classical conditioning strategies employed to create positive attitudes.
  • Also, consider how advertisers employ classical conditioning strategies to increase the desirability of their products.
  • Create a fictional product not yet on the market and design an advertisement (i.e., print, radio, television, or some other type of media ad) that uses classical conditioning strategies. Search the Internet for ideas to guide you as you design an advertisement.

Post a description of your fictional product and your advertisement. Describe the process by which classical conditioning creates favorable attitudes sufficient to encourage consumers to buy the product.

Required Readings (All attached)

Aronson, E., Wilson, T. D., Akert, R. M., & Sommers, S. R. (Eds.). (2019). Social psychology (10th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson.

  • Chapter 7, “Attitudes and Attitude Change: Influencing Thoughts and Feelings”

Staats, A. W., & Staats, C. K. (1958). Attitudes established by classical conditioning. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology57(1), 37–40. https://doiorg.ezp.waldenulibrary.org/10.1037/h0042782

Levy, N., Harmon-Jones, C., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2018). Dissonance and discomfort: Does a simple cognitive inconsistency evoke a negative affective state? Motivation Science4(2), 95–108. https://doi-org.ezp.waldenulibrary.org/10.1037/mot0000079

  • attachment

    Attitudesestablishedbyclassicalconditioning.pdf

    ATTITUDES ESTABLISHED BY CLASSICAL CONDITIONING1

    ARTHUR W. STAATS AND CAROLYN K. STAATS

    Arizona State College at Tempe

    O SGOOD and Tannenbaum have stated, “… The meaning of a concept is its location in a space denned by some

    number of factors or dimensions, and attitude toward a concept is its projection onto one of these dimensions defined as ‘evaluative’ ” (9, p. 42). Thus, attitudes evoked by concepts are considered part of the total meaning of the concepts. Discussion 1: Attitude Formation

    A number of psychologists, such as Cofer and Foley (1), Mowrer (5), and Osgood (6, 7), to mention a few, view meaning as a response—an implicit response with cue functions which may mediate other responses. A very similar analysis has been made of the concept of attitudes by Doob, who states, ” ‘An attitude is an implicit response . . . which is considered socially significant in the individual’s society’ ” (2, p. 144). Doob further emphasizes the learned character of attitudes and states, “The learning process, therefore, is crucial to an understanding of the behavior of attitudes” (2, p. 138). If attitudes are to be considered responses, then the learning process should be the same as for other responses. As an example, the principles of classical conditioning should apply to attitudes. Discussion 1: Attitude Formation

    The present authors (12), in three experi- ments, recently conditioned the evaluative, potency, and activity components of word meaning found by Osgood and Suci (8) to contiguously presented nonsense syllables. The results supported the conception that meaning is a response and, further, indicated that word meaning is composed of components which can be separately conditioned.

    The present study extends the original experiments by studying the formation of attitudes (evaluative meaning) to socially significant verbal stimuli through classical con- ditioning. The socially significant verbal stimuli were national names and familiar masculine names. Both of these types of

    1 This study is part of a series of studies of verbal behavior being conducted by the authors at Arizona State College at Tempe, The project is sponsored by the Office of Naval Research (Contract Number NONR- 2305 (00)), Arthur W. Staats, principal investigator.

    stimuli, unlike nonsense syllables, would be expected to evoke attitudinal responses on the basis of the pre-experimental experience of the 5s. Thus, the purpose of the present study is to test the hypothesis that attitudes already elicited by socially significant verbal stimuli can be changed through classical conditioning, using other words as unconditioned stimuli.

    METHOD

    Subjects Ninety-three students in elementary psychology

    participated in the experiments as 5s to fulfill a course requirement.

    Procedure The general procedure employed was the same as in

    the previous study of the authors (12). Experiment I,—The procedures were administered

    to the 5s in groups. There were two groups with one half of the 5s in each group. Two types of stimuli were used: national names which were presented by slide pro- jection on a screen (CS words) and words which were presented orally by the E (US words), with 5s required to repeat the word aloud immediately after E had pronounced it. Ostensibly, 5s’ task was to separately learn the verbal stimuli simultaneously presented in the two different ways.

    Two tasks were first presented to train the 5s in the procedure and to orient them properly for the phase of the experiment where the hypotheses were tested. The first task was to learn five visually presented national names, each shown four times, in random order. 5s’ learning was tested by recall. The second task was to learn 33 auditorily presented words. 5s repeated each word aloud after E. 5s were tested by presenting 12 pairs of words. One of each pair was a word that had just been presented, and 5s were to recognize which one.

    The 5s were then told that the primary purpose of the experiment was to study “how both of these types of learning take place together—the effect that one has upon the other, and so on.” Six new national names were used for visual presentation: German, Swedish, Italian, French, Dutch, and Greek served as the C5s.

    These names were presented in random order, with exposures of five sec. Approximately one sec. after the CS name appeared on the screen, E pronounced the US word with which it was paired. The intervals between exposures were less than one sec. 5s were told they could learn the visually presented names by just looking at them but that they should simultaneously concentrate on pronouncing the auditorily presented words aloud and to themselves, since there would be many of these words, each presented only once.

    37

     

     

    38 ARTHUR W. STAATS AND CAROLYN K. STAATS

    The names were each visually presented 18 times in random order, though never more than twice in succession, so that no systematic associations were formed between them. On each presentation, the CS name was paired with a different auditorily presented word, i.e., there were 18 conditioning trials. CS names were never paired with US words more than once so that stable associations were not formed between them. Thus, 108 different US words were used. The CS names, Swedish and Dutch, were always paired with US words with evaluative meaning. The other four CS names were paired with words which had no systematic meaning, e.g., chair, with, twelve. For Group 1, Dutch was paired with different words which had positive evaluative meaning, e.g., gift, sacred, happy; and Swedish was paired with words which had negative evaluative meaning, e.g., bitter, ugly, failure,2 For Group 2, the order of Dutch and Swedish was reversed so that Dutch was paired with words with negative evaluative meaning and Swedish with positive meaning words. Discussion 1: Attitude Formation

    When the conditioning phase was completed, 5s were told that E first wished to find out how many of the visually presented words they remembered. At the same time, they were told, it would be necessary to find out how they/eW about the words since that might have affected how the words were learned. Each S was given a small booklet in which there were six pages. On each page was printed one of the six names and a semantic differential scale. The scale was the seven- point scale of Osgood and Suci (8), with the con- tinuum from pleasant to unpleasant. An example is as follows:

    German pleasant: : : : : : : .’unpleasant

    The 5s were told how to mark the scale and to indicate at the bottom of the page whether or not the word was one that had been presented.

    The 5s were then tested on the auditorily presented words. Finally, they were asked to write down anytlu’ng they had thought about the experiment, especially the purpose of it, and so on, or anything they had thought of during the experiment. It was explained that this might have affected the way they had learned.

    Experiment //.—The procedure was exactly re- peated with another group of 5s except for the CS names. The names used were Harry, Tom, Jim, Ralph, Bill, and Bob. Again, half of the 5s were in Group 1 and half in Group 2. For Group 1, Tom was paired with positive evaluative words and Bill with negative words. For Group 2 this was reversed. The semantic differential booklet was also the same except for the C5 names.

    Design The data for the two experiments were treated in the

    same manner. Three variables were involved in the 2 The complete list of CS-US word pairs is not pre-

    sented here, but it has been deposited with the American Documentation Institute. Order Document No. 5463 from ADI Auxiliary Publications Project, Photo- duplication Service, Library of Congress, Washington 25, D. C., remitting in advance §1.25 for microfilm or SI.25 for photocopies. Make checks payable to Chief, Photoduphcation Service, Library of Congress. Discussion 1: Attitude Formation

    design: conditioned meaning (pleasant and unpleasant); C5 names (Dutch and Swedish, or Tom and Bill); and groups (1 and 2). The scores on the semantic differential given to each of the two CS words were analyzed in a 2 x 2 latin square as described by Lindquist (4, p. 278) for his Type II design.

    RESULTS

    The 17 5s who indicated they were aware of either of the systematic name-word relation- ships were excluded from the analysis. This was done to prevent the interpretation that the conditioning of attitudes depended upon awareness. In order to maintain a counter- balanced design when these 5s were excluded, four 5s were randomly eliminated from the analysis. The resulting Ns were as follows: 24 in Experiment I and 48 in Experiment II.

    Table 1 presents the means and SDs of the meaning scores for Experiments I and II. The table itself is a representation of the 2 X 2 design for each experiment. The pleasant

    TABLE 1

    MEANS AND SDs OF CONDITIONED ATTITUDE SCORES

    Names

    Dutchum^u Swedish Expen- ment Group Mean SD Mean SD

    I 1 2

    2.67 2.67

    .94 1.31

    3.42 1.83

    1.50 .90

    Tom Bill

    ment II

    Group 1 2

    Mean

    2.71 3.42

    SD

    2.01 2.55

    Mean

    4.12 1.79

    SD

    2.04 1.07

    Note.—On the scales, pleasant is I, unpleasant 7.

    TABLE 2

    SUMMARY OF THE RESULTS OF THE ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE FOR EACH EXPERIMENT

    Source

    Between 5s Groups Error

    Within Conditioned

    attitude Names Residual

    Total

    Exp. I

    df

    1 22

    1

    1 22 47

    MS

    7.52 1.73

    7.52

    .02 1.36

    F

    4.36*

    5.52*

    .01

    Exp. II

    df

    1 46

    1

    1 46 95

    MS

    15.84 3.17

    55.51

    .26 5.30

    F

    5.00*

    10.47**

    .05

    ‘ t < .05. “p < .01.

     

     

    ATTITUDES ESTABLISHED BY CLASSICAL CONDITIONING 39

    extreme of the evaluative scale was scored 1, the unpleasant 7.

    The analysis of the data for both experi- ments is presented in Table 2, The results of the analysis indicate that the conditioning occurred in both cases. In Experiment I, the F for the conditioned attitudes was significant at better than the .05 level. In Experiment II, the F for the conditioned attitudes was signifi- cant at better than the .01 level. In both experiments the F for the groups variable was significant at the .05 level.

    DISCUSSION

    It was possible to condition the attitude component of the total meaning responses of US words to socially significant verbal stimuli, without Ss’ awareness. This conception is schematized in Fig. 1, and in so doing, the way the conditioning in this study was thought to have taken place is shown more specifically. The national name Dutch, in this example, is presented prior to the word pretty. Pretty elicits a meaning response. This is schematized in the figure as two component responses; an evaluative response rpy (in this example, the words have a positive value), and the other distinctive responses that characterize the meaning of the word, Rp. The pairing of Dutch and pretty results in associations between Dutch and rpv, and Dutch and Rp. In the fol- lowing presentations of Dutch and the words sweet and healthy, the association between Dutch and rPv is further strengthened. This is not the case with associations RP, Rs, and RH,

    CS

    DUTCH ^.-=u-_ _ .

    “PRETTY.

    DUTCH

    -HEAI/DHY

    FIG. 1. THE CONDITIONING or A POSITIVE ATTITUDE. THE HEAVINESS OF LINE REPRESENTS STRENGTH

    OP ASSOCIATION

    since they occur only once and are followed by other associations which are inhibitory. The direct associations indicated in the figure between the name and the individual words would also in this way be inhibited.

    It was not thought that a rating response was conditioned in this procedure but rather an implicit attitudinal response which medi- ated the behavior of scoring the semantic differential scale. It is possible, with this con- ception, to interpret two studies by Razran (10, 11) which concern the conditioning of rat- ings. Razran found that ratings of ethnically labeled pictures of girls and sociopolitical slo- gans could be changed by showing these stimuli while Ss were consuming a free lunch and, in the case of the slogans, while the 5s were presented with unpleasant olfactory stimula- tion. The change in ratings could be thought to be due to the conditioning of an implicit evaluative response, an attitude, to the CSs by means of the lunch or the unpleasant odors. That is, part of the total response elicited by the food, for example, was conditioned to the pictures or slogans and became the mediation process which in turn elicited the positive rating.

    It should be stated that the results of the present study do not show directly that Ss’ behavior to the object (e.g., a person of Dutch nationality) has been changed. The results pertain to the Ss’ attitudinal response to the signs, the national names themselves. However, Kapustnik (3) has demonstrated that a re- sponse generalized to an object when the re- sponse had previously been conditioned to the verbal sign of the object. Osgood states,

    The aggressive reactions associated with Nazi and Jap on a verbal level certainly transferred to the social objects represented under appropriate conditions. Similarly, prejudicial behaviors established while read- ing about a member of a social class can transfer to the class as a whole . . . (7, p. 704).

    The results of this study have special rele- vance for an understanding of attitude forma- tion and change by means of verbal communi- cation. Using a conception of meaning as a mediating response, Mowrer (5) has suggested that a sentence is a conditioning device and that communication takes place when the meaning response which has been elicited by the predicate is conditioned to the subject of the sentence. The results of the present study and the previous one of the present authors

     

     

    40 ARTHUR W. STAATS AND CAROLYN K. STAATS

    (12) substantiate Mowrer’s approach by sub- stantiating the basic theory that word meaning will indeed condition to contiguously pre- sented verbal stimuli. In the present study, the meaning component was evaluative, or attitudinal, and the CSs were socially signifi- cant verbal stimuli. The results suggest, there- fore, that attitude formation or change through communication takes place according to these principles of conditioning. As an example, the sentence, “Dutch people are honest,” would condition the positive attitude elicited by “honest” to “Dutch”—and presumably to any person called “Dutch,” If, in an individual’s history, many words eliciting a positive atti- tude were paired with “Dutch,” then a very positive attitude toward this nationality would arise.

    The reason for the group differences in each of the experiments is not clear. These differ- ences could have arisen because there were actual differences in the 5″s composing each group, or in some condition of the procedure occurring to one of the groups. Nothing the authors were aware of seem to indicate this as the explanation, and in the previous experi- ments of the authors (12) there were no group differences. Since in a 2 x 2 latin square the interactions are entirely confounded with the main effects, the group differences could also have arisen as a result of the interaction of the other two main effects (i.e., direction of con- ditioning and names). Discussion 1: Attitude Formation

    SUMMARY

    Two experiments were conducted to test the hypothesis that attitude responses elicited by a word can be conditioned to a contigu- ously presented socially significant verbal stimulus. A name (e.g., Dutch) was presented 18 times, each time paired with the auditory presentation of a different word. While these

    words were different, they all had an identical evaluative meaning component. In Experi- ment I, one national name was paired with positive evaluative meaning and another was paired with negative evaluative meaning. In Experiment II, familiar masculine names were used. In each experiment there was sig- nificant evidence that meaning responses had been conditioned to the names without 5s’ awareness.

    REFERENCES

    1. COEER, C. N., & FOLEY, J. P. Mediated generaliza- tion and the interpretation of verbal behavior: I. Prologemena. Psychol. Rev., 1942, 49, 513- 540.

    2. DOOB, L. VV. The behavior of attitudes. Psychol. Rev., 1947, 54, 135-156.

    3. KAPUSTNIK, 0. P. The interrelation between direct conditioned stimuli and their verbal symbols. (Trans, from Russian title) Psychol. Abstr., 1934, 8, No. 153.

    4. LINDQUIST, E. F. Design and analysis oj exper- iments in psychology and education. Boston: Hough ton Mifflin, 1953.

    5. MOWRER, O. H. The psychologist looks at lan- guage. Amer. Psychologist, 1954, 9, 660-694.

    6. OSGOOD, C. E. The nature and measurement of meaning. Psychol. Brill, 1952, 49, 197-237.

    7. OSGOOD, C. E. Method and theory in experimental psychology. New York: Oxford Univer. Press, 1953.

    8. OSGOOD, C. E., & Suci, G. J. Factor analysis of meaning. J. exp. Psychol., 1955, 60, 325-338.

    9. OSGOOD, C. E., & TANNENBAUM, P. H. The principle of congruity in the prediction of attitude change. Psychol. Rev., 1955, 62, 42-55.

    10. RAZRAN, G. H. S. Conditioning away social bias by the luncheon technique. Psychol. Bidl., 1938, 35, 693.

    11. RAZRAN, G. H. S. Conditioned response changes in rating and appraising sociopolitical slogans. Psychol. Bidl, 1940, 37, 481.

    12. STAATS, C. K., & STAATS, A. W. Meaning estab- lished by classical conditioning. /. exp. Psychol, 1957, B4, 74-80.

    Received June 12, 1957.

  • attachment

    DissonanceandDiscomfortDoesaSimpleCognitiveInconsistency.pdf
  • attachment

    Chapter7AttitudesandAttitudeChange.pdf