Discussion 1 Paper: Schemas
Discussion 1 Paper: Schemas
Most everyone has seen criminal trials (both fictitious and real) on TV. You know, then, that the trial starts with opening statements and ends with closing arguments by the prosecution and defense, respectively.
For this Discussion, you will examine the advantages and disadvantages of schemas.
ORDER NOW FOR COMPREHENSIVE, PLAGIARISM-FREE PAPERS
To Prepare
- Review the Learning Resources for this week and consider the advantages and disadvantages of the schema.
- Imagine you are a juror. Exactly how do opening statements and closing arguments function as schemas? In particular, what impact would opening statements have on how you, as a juror, would interpret evidence proffered at trial?
Post how your preconceptions (i.e., your schema) might supersede the schema (i.e., theory) of the case as presented by attorneys for the state and for the defendant during opening statements. How might the strength of your preconceptions cause you to reject the attorneys’ attempt to provide a schema for the case?
During jury selection, attorneys for the prosecution and defense question prospective jurors to determine who may already have decided the defendant’s guilt or innocence before hearing the attorneys present the case. As an attorney, it is your job to expose and then counter juror preconceptions that may make them resistant to your schema for the case. Your class colleagues have described how their preconceptions might override the schema you, as an attorney, would advance during opening statements. Describe the strategy you might use to persuade the resistant juror (i.e., the class colleague you are responding to) to set aside her or his preconceptions and consider what you have to say. Discussion 1 Paper: Schemas
Be sure to support your postings and responses with specific references to the social psychology theory and research. In addition to the Learning Resources, search the Walden Library and/or Internet for peer-reviewed articles to support your post and responses. Use proper APA format and citations, including those in the Learning Resources.
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=ghNlOsy5DBQ
Aronson, E., Wilson, T. D., Akert, R. M., & Sommers, S. R. (Eds.). (2019). Social psychology (10th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson.
- Chapter 3, “Social Cognition: How We Think About the Social World”
-
Standardformconotractsandcontractschemas.pdf
Standard Form Contracts and Contract Schemas: A Preliminary Investigation of the Effects of Exculpatory Clauses on Consumers’ Propensity to Sue
Dennis P. Stolle, M.A., J.D.,1* and Andrew J. Slain, B.S.1*
This study investigated the extent to which exculpatory
clauses deter consumers from pursuing their legal rights.
Undergraduate participants (N=101) were presented with
two written vignettes and asked to imagine themselves as a
consumer harmed by a contracted for service. Participants
then read a contract and responded to questions assessing
their likelihood of seeking compensation and their
perceptions of the contract. The presence of exculpatory
clauses, the severity of the harm, and the nature of the
harm were varied. The data suggest that exculpatory
clauses, if read, have a deterrent effect on propensity to
seek compensation. Development of a psychological
definition of contract schemas and implications for legal
policy are discussed. # 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Behav. Sci. Law Vol. 15, 83±94 1997.
No. of Figures: 0. No. of Tables: 1. No. of Refs: 27.
In 1963, Stewart Macaulay asked a few empirical questions about contract law:
“What good is contract law? who uses it? when and how?” (p. 55). Using survey
and interview methodologies, Macaulay set out to answer those questions.
Interestingly, Macaulay found that formal contract doctrine often takes a back
seat to extra-legal conceptions of fair dealing and “common honesty and decency”
(p. 58). Macaulay’s heavy reliance on empirical data was viewed by some legal
scholars as contributing to the demise of traditional contract theory and doctrine.
CCC 0735±3936/97/010083±12$17.50 #1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Behavioral Sciences and the Law, Vol. 15, 83±94 (1997) RESEARCH REPORT
* Correspondence to: either author at 1University of Nebraska-Lincoln, 209 Burnett Hall, Lincoln, Nebraska 68588-0308 USA. Electronic mail may be directed to the first author at dstolle@unlgrad1.unl.edu or the second author at aslain@unlgrad1.unl.edu.
Dennis P. Stolle, M.A., J.D. (expected May 1997), and Andrew J. Slain, B.A., are J.D.-Ph.D. students in the Law/Psychology Program at the University Nebraska-Lincoln. The authors are grateful to professors Mark Fondacaro, Steven Penrod, Alan Tomkins, and Robert Works for their helpful suggestions and comments, and Michelle Dreesen for her assistance in collecting and coding the data. A version of this article was presented at the biennial conference of the American Psychology-Law Society, Hilton Head, South Carolina, February, 1996.
This research was made possible in part by a University of Nebraska-Lincoln Warden Research Grant awarded to the first author. This article was completed while the first author was in receipt of a National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) Fellowship. The financial support of NIMH is gratefully acknowledged.
Macaulay was even dubbed the “Lord High Executioner of the Contract is Dead
School” (Gilmore, 1974, p. 105). Nonetheless, Macaulay’s findings did not toll the
death knell for contract doctrine and theory. Rather, they offered a new and more
empirically accurate way of conceptualizing contracts.
In the years since Macaulay’s initial work, Law and Society researchers have
continued to offer empirical views of contractual behavior that challenge the
assumptions of traditional contract doctrine (Macaulay, 1985). Although the Law
and Society research has produced findings relevant to both legal and sociolegal
scholars, the research has not emphasized inquiry into the implicit psychology of
contract. Furthermore, the Social Science in Law movement, which places a strong
emphasis on using psychological theory to “sharpen the legal scholar’s insights”
(Monahan & Walker, 1993, p. v), has been largely absent from the empirical
contract literature. Perhaps the most psychologically sophisticated studies of
contractual behaviors are those studies investigating the importance of the
“psychological contract” in employment contexts (e.g., Morrison & Robinson, in
press; Robinson, in press; Robinson & Rousseau, 1994; Rousseau, 1989; Rousseau
& Aquino, 1993; Schmedemann & McLean Parks, 1994). However, most of the
employment studies emphasize the management implications of the psychological
contract over the legal implications. Discussion 1 Paper: Schemas
Altogether there exists little more than a handful of empirical studies exploring
the psycholegal dimensions of contract (e.g., Masson & Waldron, 1994). Indeed,
numerous legal scholars have noted the need for sophisticated empirical research
and the application of psychological theory in the context of contract law
(Eisenberg, 1995; Harrison, 1994; Hasen, 1990; MacNeil, 1985; Rubin, 1995;
Speidel, 1995; Stratman, 1988; White, 1988). As the domain of contract widens
and shifts, it has become clear that to truly understand the nature of contract,
“empirical theories dealing with the use and abuse of contract behavior in the
shadow of contract law and beyond will be required” (Speidel, 1995, p. 255). The
purpose of the present study was to provide a preliminary attempt at exploring
empirical theories of contract from a psycholegal perspective.
THE PRESENCE OF EXCULPATORY CLAUSES IN FORM CONTRACTS
The vast majority of consumer transactions are conducted via standardized
agreements presented to consumers on preprinted forms with little or no
opportunity for the consumer to negotiate the terms of the agreement. The
widespread use of standard form contracts is largely a result of their efficiency
(Farnsworth, 1990). Because the costs involved in negotiating individual contracts
would often exceed the potential profit arising from many routine transactions,
standard form contracts are an essential element of modern commercial life
(Farnsworth, 1990).
Unfortunately, traditional contract doctrine, which contemplates a bargained-for
exchange between parties of relatively equal power, often falls short of providing an
adequate analytic framework for resolving disputes involving standard form
contracts (Farnsworth, 1991). Form contracts seldom involve parties of equal
bargaining power, they are typically offered on a take-it-or-leave-it basis, and the
84 D. P. Stolle and A. J. Slain
terms embodied in such forms sometimes seek to alter the base-line legal rights of
the consumer (Eisenberg, 1995). For example, it is not uncommon for form
contracts to include terms that, if enforced, would relieve the drafting party of
liability for their own negligence. The enforceability of such terms, commonly
referred to as exculpatory clauses, is often unclear (Morant, 1995). Many
jurisdictions have held these types of disclaimers unenforceable because the
terms are either unconscionable, in violation of public policy, or are beyond
the range of the consumer’s reasonable expectations (Morant, 1995). Although the
enforceability of exculpatory clauses is often suspect, they remain commonplace in
consumer form contracts. Discussion 1 Paper: Schemas
Some commentators have suggested that the knowing inclusion of unenforceable
disclaimers is unethical because it creates a facade of legality that may deter
consumers from bringing otherwise legitimate claims against the drafting party
(Kuklin, 1988; Vukowich, 1993). In response to such concerns, the discussion
draft of the Model Rules of Professional Conduct contained a provision prohibiting
lawyers from drafting agreements containing “legally prohibited terms,” or terms
that “would be held to be unconscionable as a matter of law” (Vukowich, 1993,
p. 776). However, strong objections from the legal community to a rule that would
penalize the inclusion of overly broad disclaimers caused the rule to be eliminated,
leaving the current Model Rules without any reference to a lawyer’s responsibility
as a contract drafter (Vukowich, 1993). At least part of the reasoning behind the
objections was that a rule restricting the terms that a lawyer may include in a
contract would infringe upon the lawyer’s ability to fully protect his client’s
interests under a broad range of possible contingencies.
The current state of the common law is such that the inclusion of exculpatory
clauses is prohibited only under limited circumstances. Furthermore, the
enforceability of exculpatory clauses varies greatly between jurisdictions, and as a
function of the specific circumstances, leaving the consumer with little ability to
predict whether any particular disclaimer clause will be enforceable (Morant,
1995). Of course, the extent to which exculpatory clauses actually deter consumers
from bringing legal actions is an empirical question. If such terms do have an effect
on consumers’ propensity to sue, it seems likely that those effects may interact with
other variables, such as the type and severity of the harm suffered by the consumer.
If such terms in fact have no effect on consumers’ propensity to pursue their legal
rights, then some of the concern regarding the presence of such clauses in consumer
form contracts may be unwarranted. Discussion 1 Paper: Schemas
CONTRACT SCHEMAS AND EXCULPATORY CLAUSES
In the present context, a discussion of the effects of exculpatory language on
consumer’s propensity to sue is largely a discussion of “expectations and their
effects” (Fiske & Taylor, 1991, p. 97). Such expectations may form the basis of
cognitive schemasГgeneric knowledge that holds across many particular
instances” (Fiske & Taylor, 1991, p. 98). Indeed, researchers have found support
for the notion of contract schemas in the employment context (Schmedemann &
McLean Parks, 1994; Morrison & Robinson, in press). Like employees, consumers
Standard form contracts 85
may base some of their expectations about contractual transactions upon cognitive
schemas.
Schmedemann and McLean Parks suggest that contract schemas may include
such attributes as legal jargon and the presence of a signature block (1994). Early
empirical work on contract disclaimers suggests that consumers’ contract schemas
may also include a belief that terms in a written contract are generally enforceable.
In 1970, Warren Mueller conducted a study to examine tenant perceptions of
residential leases (1970). Mueller found that most participants in his study believed
that the exculpatory clauses presented in the mock lease would be enforceable and
that there would be little variation between states in terms of enforceability (1970).
These beliefs were erroneous. In fact, Mueller had presented his participants with
clauses unlikely to be upheld in the state where the study was conducted (1970).
Furthermore, at the time the study was conducted, there was wide variability
among states in the enforceability of the clauses presented.
Although Mueller’s study was not intended as an investigation of contract
schemas, his data suggests that, at least in 1970, consumers held a general belief
that provisions contained in the residential leases they sign are enforceable.
However, Mueller’s study suffers from several limitations. Mueller’s participants
responded on a dichotomous, yes/no, scale; consequently, Mueller had no
measurement of the magnitude of his participants’ beliefs. Furthermore,
Mueller’s study was conducted in a survey format with no experimental
manipulations, and Mueller’s participants were not presented with circumstances
in which the exculpatory language would be relevant. Previous schema research
suggests that increasing the costs of being wrong may decrease reliance on schemas
and increase the desire to seek out new data (Fiske & Taylor, 1991). Consequently,
a remaining question is whether participants placed in a relevant context in which
an exculpatory clause would have particularly detrimental effects on their
immediate future would abandon their contract schema and seek out new
information. In this context, methods of seeking out relevant information might
include talking to a representative of the drafting party or talking to a lawyer.
METHODS
Participants
Participants were 101 undergraduate students recruited from psychology courses at
a mid-western university. Most participants volunteered in exchange for class
credit. The final sample consisted of 51% women and 45% men; 4% of the
participants failed to indicate their gender. Participants’ mean age was 21, and the
median age was 19.
Materials
Vignettes
Two separate vignettes were utilized, one involving a personal injury and the other
involving damage to personal property. Both vignettes asked participants to
86 D. P. Stolle and A. J. Slain
imagine themselves in the place of a consumer who was harmed as a result of a
contracted for service. In the personal injury vignette, a consumer received either a
small cut or a spinal injury while using exercise equipment at a health club. In the
property damage vignette, a consumer’s car was either scratched or stolen while in
the care of an automotive repair shop. To avoid the complications of insurance,
participants were asked to imagine that they did not have relevant insurance
coverage. Discussion 1 Paper: Schemas
Both vignettes included a copy of the contract that the consumer signedÐa
membership agreement in the health club vignette and an estimate form in the
automotive repair vignette. The contract language was adopted from actual
contracts, with company names and other identifying information altered. The two
contracts were comparable in terms of length, organization, and complexity.1 The
apparent intended legal effect of the exculpatory clause in both contracts was to
relieve the drafting parties of liability even for their own negligence.2
Questionnaire
Each questionnaire contained 6 manipulation check items, 12 items measured on
7-point Likert-type scales, and two open-ended questions. The Likert-type items
were designed to assess the participants’ self-reported likelihood of seeking
compensation and reading the contract, and the participants’ perceptions of the
enforceability, fairness, and difficulty of the contract language. In order to control
for the possibility that some effects may be a function of participants’
predetermined attitudes toward car dealers and health clubs, a final Likert-type
item designed to directly assess perceptions of reputability was included. The
questionnaire items are reproduced in the Appendix.
Design and Procedure
The design was a 2 (severity: minor vs. severe) 62 (presence of exculpatory clause: present vs. not present) 62 (damage type: personal injury vs. property damage) design in which the last factor was a repeated measures variable and was used as a
conceptual replication to assess generalization across legal contexts. After
completing an informed consent procedure, each participant received a pack
containing the two contract vignettes and two questionnaires. The order in which
the vignettes appeared was randomly assigned, and the between-subjects conditions
were randomly assigned. The participants were instructed to read through the
Standard form contracts 87
1 Both contracts received a Flesch reading ease score of 23, indicating that the contracts contained difficult reading appropriate for readers at the 11±13th grade level (Felsenfeld & Siegel, 1981). Both contracts also received a Gunning’s Fog Index rating of 24. The average word length was 1.68 syllables in the health club contract and 1.72 syllables in the automotive repair contract. The average sentence length was 40.8 words and 37.9 words respectively, and the average paragraph length was 1.7 sentences and 1.5 sentences respectively. Copies of the contracts are available from the first author upon request. 2 The following is an example of the exculpatory language: Buyer specifically agrees that Gym, its officers, employees and agents shall not be liable for any claim, demand, or cause of action of any kind whatsoever for, or on account of death, personal injury, property damage or loss of any kind resulting from or related to Member’s use of the facilities or participation in any sport, exercise or activity within or without the club premises, and Buyer agrees to hold Gym harmless for same. Discussion 1 Paper: Schemas
materials and to answer the questions following each vignette. Participants were
free to refer back to the vignettes, including the written contract, as they completed
the questionnaires.
Hypotheses
Based on Mueller’s finding that tenants tend to believe that the provisions in
the leases they sign are enforceable, we reasoned that participants would rely
on a contract schema that includes a belief that terms in a written contract are
generally enforceable. Consequently, we expected to find a main effect for the
presence of an exculpatory clause such that participants assigned to conditions
in which an exculpatory clause was present would be less likely than
participants assigned to conditions in which such a clause was not present to
express a strong propensity to seek legal advice or to demand compensation on
their own behalf. Based upon schema theory, we reasoned that as the cost of
being wrong increased, participants’ reliance on their contract schema would
decrease and that participants would show a greater propensity to seek out
additional information. Consequently, we expected to find a main effect for the
severity of the harm such that participants assigned to conditions in which the
harm is minor would be less likely than participants assigned to conditions in
which the harm is severe to express a strong propensity to seek legal advice or
to demand compensation on their own behalf. Furthermore, we expected to
find an interaction between presence of an exculpatory clause and severity of
harm such that when the harm was severe, the presence or absence of an
exculpatory clause would become less relevant to participants’ propensity to
seek legal advice or to demand compensation on their own behalf. Finally, we
expected results would generalize across the two vignettes presented to the
participants. Discussion 1 Paper: Schemas
RESULTS3
Manipulation Checks
Initial analyses were performed on the manipulation check items to determine
whether participants had identified and understood the exculpatory clauses in both
the auto-repair contract and the health club contract. For the auto-repair contract,
a chi-square analysis revealed that almost two-thirds of the participants (65%) were
able to correctly identify whether or not a clause was present that may prevent their
recovery in a lawsuit, w2 (1, N=99)=8.49, p=0.004. Furthermore, most participants (82%) who correctly indicated that a clause was present were able to correctly
88 D. P. Stolle and A. J. Slain
3 Prior to analysis, each item was examined for accuracy of data entry, missing values, and fit between their distributions and the assumptions of multivariate analysis. Square root transformations were performed on four of the items to address their unacceptable skewness. With the transformed variables in the variable set, one case was identified through Mahalanobis distance as a multivariate outlier and was deleted from the data set. Discussion 1 Paper: Schemas
identify the paragraph containing the exculpatory clause, w2 (5, N=53)=41.98, p50.001.4
For the health club contract, a chi-square analysis again revealed that almost
two-thirds of the participants (66%) were able to correctly identify whether or not a
clause was present that may prevent their recovery in a lawsuit, w2 (1, N=100)=10.35, p50.001. Furthermore, an examination of the presence of clause by paragraph cross-classification table revealed that of those participants
who correctly indicated a clause was present over 70% correctly identified
paragraph five as containing the clause. However, twelve participants in conditions
containing no relevant exculpatory language also indicated that paragraph five
contained relevant exculpatory language. These false positive identifications led to
a non-significant chi-square statistic, w2 (6, N=58)=6.97, p=0.32.5
Principal Components Analysis
A principal components factor analysis with varimax rotation was performed on
all subjects’ responses to the 12 Likert-type questions (collapsed across the eight
conditions, N=100). The purpose of this procedure was to simplify analysis and clarify measurements by developing a small set of uncorrelated components
representing the larger number of questionnaire items. Four components were
extracted based upon the eigenvalue greater than one criteria. However, an
examination of the initial statistics revealed that a fifth component had an
eigenvalue of 0.95. Consequently, a five component solution was attempted.
When five components were extracted, the fifth component was unique, with
only the reputability item loading on that component. To be considered a part of a
component, an item was required to have a loading of 0.64 on only that
component. This five component solution was retained because it increased the
interpretability of the components by separating the reputability item, which was
intended to assess predetermined attitudes toward car dealers and health clubs
rather than to assess the effects of the experimental manipulations. The five
components accounted for 74.7% of the total measurement variance. The
constituent items and their loadings are presented in Table 1.
The labeled components in order of percentage of explained variance were 1)
propensity to seek compensation, 2) likelihood of reading, 3) fairness/readability, 4)
similarity/enforceability, and 5) reputability. It was reasoned that propensity to seek
compensation, fairness, and similarity/enforceability would be significantly related
to the variables manipulated in the vignettes. In contrast, it was reasoned that the
experimental manipulations would be unlikely to effect likelihood of reading, and
Standard form contracts 89
4 Interestingly, among those participants who mistakenly identified an exculpatory clause when none was present, 68% identified paragraph 3 as containing the exculpatory clause. Paragraph 3 of the auto- repair contract did contain disclaimers regarding a parts warranty, perhaps making paragraph 3 the best choice in the absence of the experimental exculpatory clause. However, the warranty language was not legally relevant under the circumstances described to the participants. 5 Paragraph five did contain some exculpatory type language even when the experimental clause was not present. However, the remaining exculpatory language was not relevant under the circumstances described. Yet, the language may have appeared relevant to the naive legal actor, accounting for the false±positive identifications. Discussion 1 Paper: Schemas
that any difference in perceptions of reputability would likely be the result of
predetermined attitudes toward car dealers and health clubs. To test these
hypotheses, a new set of composite variables was created to represent the
components, each variable being the average of the set of variables that loaded on a
component.
Analysis of Variance
An ANOVA was performed on each of the orthogonal composite variables, with
follow-up analyses of simple effects where appropriate. First, regarding the effects
of presence of an exculpatory clause, analysis of variance revealed a main effect
such that when the clause was present participants indicated that they would be less
likely to demand compensation or seek legal advice than when the clause was not
present, F(1,95)=4.09, p=0.046, Z2=0.04. Second, regarding the effects of severity of the harm, analysis of variance revealed
a significant interaction between severity of the harm and damage type for
propensity to seek compensation, F(1,95)=4.75, p=0.032, Z2=0.05. Analysis of simple effects for propensity to seek compensation revealed that in the property
damage vignette participants indicated that they would be more likely to seek
compensation when the harm was severe (M=3.37) than when the harm was minor (M=2.95), F(1,98)=5.18, p=0.025, Z2=0.05. Furthermore, when the harm was minor, participants indicated that they would be more likely to seek compensation
in the personal injury vignette (M=3.31) than in the property damage vignette (M=2.95), F(1,50)=3.21, p=0.057, Z2=0.06.
Finally, regarding the effects of damage type, analysis of variance revealed a
significant main effect such that participants indicated that in general health clubs
are more reputable than auto dealerships, F(1,95)=44.18, p50.001, Z2=0.32.
90 D. P. Stolle and A. J. Slain
Table 1. Results of the principal components analysis on contract questionnaire items
Component Variance (%)
Constituent Items Loadings
1. Propensity to seek 23.6 Probability that a lawsuit would be successful. 0.87 compensation Probability that demand will be successful. 0.84
Likelihood of demanding payment for losses. 0.76 Likelihood of seeking advice from an attorney. 0.69
2. Likelihood of reading 20.7 Likelihood of reading the agreement. 0.91 contract Likelihood of reading the agreement carefully. 0.88
3. Fairness and 12.5 Likelihood that contract violates legal rights. 0.79 Readability Difficulty understanding the contract
language. 0.70
Fairness of the contract as a whole. 0.68 4. Similarity and 10.1 Similarity of contract to those commonly used. 0.81
Enforceability Likelihood a court would enforce the contract. 0.64 5. Reputability 7.9 Reputability of most garages/gyms in general. 0.95
Total 74.7
Qualitative Responses
The qualitative results allowed for an examination of more psychologically rich
responses. The first open-ended question provides some additional insight into
participants’ initial reaction to the events described in the vignettes. A total of 94
out of 100 participants answered the following question: “What would you do in
similar circumstances?” Three coders independently categorized the responses with
an inter-rater reliability of 87%.6 Collapsed across all of the conditions, participants
indicated that their likely courses of action might include seeking legal help,
engaging in self-help, absorbing the costs themselves, ending their business
relationship with the drafting party, or seeking help from insurance agents, police,
friends, or family. However, most responses indicated an intention to either seek
legal help (46%) or engage in self-help (29%). Discussion 1 Paper: Schemas
DISCUSSION
The primary focus of this study was the effect of exculpatory clauses on consumers’
propensity to seek compensation when harmed in a setting governed by a form
contract. As indicated by the qualitative results, participants’ initial reaction to the
described situations was to seek compensation, either through legal representation
or by dealing directly with the representatives of the business. However, consistent
with the concerns of many legal scholars, the presence of exculpatory language did
have a deterrent effect on participants’ propensity to seek compensation. This main
effect for presence of an exculpatory clause is also consistent with previous research
suggesting that consumers’ contract schema includes a general belief that written
contract terms are enforceable (Mueller, 1970), thus accounting for their decreased
propensity to seek compensation in the face of an exculpatory clause. Surprisingly,
the presence of an exculpatory clause did not impact participants’ perceptions of
the fairness of the contracts.
We expected that when the costs of being wrong were great (i.e., the harm was
severe), participants would be less likely to rely on their contract schemas (Fiske &
Taylor, 1991). This hypothesis received partial support. There was no main effect
for severity of the harm. However, a significant interaction between damage type
and severity of the harm revealed that in the property damage vignette participants
indicated a greater propensity to seek compensation when the harm was severe than
when the harm was minor. In contrast, severity of the harm had no effect on
propensity to seek compensation in the personal injury vignette. Furthermore,
participants indicated a greater propensity to seek compensation when the harm
was minor in the personal injury vignette than in the property damage vignette.
These effects may be the result of participants having a stronger affective reaction to
Standard form contracts 91
6 The first coder grouped the responses in a manner such that seven representative categories developed. Using the category definitions developed by the first coder, two more coders independently categorized the responses. The assessment of inter-rater reliability was calculated by dividing the number of agreements by the number of agreements plus disagreements between each possible pairing of researchers on a large sample of the coded data. The three percentages of agreement between the three indepedent coders (84%, 90%, 86%) were then averaged to produce the overall inter-rater reliability of 87%. Discussion 1 Paper: Schemas
personal injuries than to property damage, leading them to be more likely to seek
compensation for a personal injury regardless of the specific circumstances.
The type of damage repeated measures variable was included to determine
whether the effects of presence of an exculpatory clause and severity of the harm
would generalize across contexts. Thus, we did not expect any main effects of type
of damage for propensity to seek compensation. However, because the property
damage condition involved a car dealership and the personal injury condition
involved a health club, we were concerned that participants may bring
predetermined attitudes regarding the reputability of car dealers and health
clubs. Rather than allow this potential confound, we directly measured
participants’ attitudes towards car dealerships and health clubs. Consistent with
our concerns, participants rated health clubs as being more reputable than car
dealerships. However, the only main effect of damage type was for reputability,
suggesting that participants’ attitudes towards car dealerships and health clubs had
little effect on their ratings on the other dependent variables. Discussion 1 Paper: Schemas
Limitations and Implications
This study represents a novel, but preliminary, attempt at expanding empirical
psychological inquiry into an area of contract law that has been relatively untapped
by psycholegal researchers. Although it is quite likely that undergraduates are
regularly exposed to the types of contracts presented in our vignettes, the use of
undergraduate participants and a vignette methodology does limit the
generalizability of these results. Replications with differing methods and
populations are needed. Furthermore, the high number of false positive
identifications of exculpatory language revealed in the manipulation checks
suggest that the participants had considerable difficulty comprehending the
contractual language,7 and this may have contributed to the relatively small
effect size (n2=0.04) for presence of the clause. Such difficulties are not inconsistent with previous findings (Masson & Waldron, 1994) and may be exacerbated in
research using less well educated samples. It must also be remembered that many
consumers may not read form contracts at the time of the transaction.8
Furthermore, it can be assumed that not all consumers would refer back to the
language of their contract after incurring an injury as a result of the contracted for
service. Discussion 1 Paper: Schemas
Although preliminary, the present data do raise several interesting implications
for the ongoing legal debate over the inclusion of potentially unenforceable
exculpatory clauses and the ongoing development of psychological theories of
contractual behaviors. First, consistent with the concerns of many legal scholars,
the presence of exculpatory language in form contracts does appear to have some
deterrent effect on consumers’ propensity to seek compensation. Second,
consistent with the findings of Schmedemann and McLean Parks (1994), the
92 D. P. Stolle and A. J. Slain
7 Participants in this study indicated that the language contained in the contracts was “somewhat difficult” to understand (M=3.8). 8 Participants in this study indicated that they would be “somewhat likely” to read the contract (M=4.00) but would be less likely to read the contract closely (M=3.53), t(99)=4.51, p50.001.
data supports the notion of a contract schema. Third, consistent with the findings
of Mueller (1970), the data suggest that consumers’ contract schemas may include
a general belief that all contract terms are enforceable. Finally, the results suggest
that in the face of increased costs of being wrong, consumers may abandon their
contract schema. In cases involving damage to property, when the severity of the
harm was great participants showed a greater propensity to seek compensation
rather than rely on their general belief in the enforceability of contract terms.
Additional research should further investigate the nature and content of contract
schemas and their impact on decision making across a variety of subject
populations and transactional contexts. Discussion 1 Paper: Schemas
APPENDIX
PLEASE ANSWER THE FOLLOWING QUESTIONS.
FEEL FREE TO REFER BACK TO THE SCENARIO
YOU JUST READ.
1. What would you do in similar circumstances? Please explain briefly:
2. How similar do you think this [agreement/estimate] is to those used by most
[health clubs/garages]?
3. How difficult do you find the language of the [membership/estimate] form to
understand?
4. Under the circumstances described above, how likely do you think you
would have been to read the agreement?
5. Under the circumstances described above, how likely do you think you
would have been to read the agreement closely?
6. How likely would you be to demand that the [health club/garage] pay for
your losses?
7. How successful do you think you would be if you demanded that the [health
club/garage] pay for your losses?
8. Do you think that there are any clauses in the [membership/estimate] form
that might prevent you from recovering on your demand?
8a. If so, please circle the number of the paragraph(s) that might prevent
your recovery:
9. How likely would you be to seek advice from an attorney?
10. Assuming the attorney advised you a lawsuit was possible, how successful do
you think your lawsuit might be?
11. Do you think that there are any clauses in the [membership/estimate] form
that might prevent you from recovering in a lawsuit?
11a. If so, please circle the number of the paragraph(s) that might prevent
your recovery:
12. How likely do you think it is that the [membership/estimate] form you signed
violates your legal rights?
13. How likely do you think it is that a court of law would uphold the
[membership/estimate] form that you signed?
14. How fair do you think the [membership/estimate] form as a whole is?
Standard form contracts 93
15. Do you think that any of the paragraphs in the [membership/estimate] form
are unethical?
15a. If so, please circle the number of the paragraph(s) that might be
unethical:
16. Have you ever had an experience similar to that in this scenario? Explain:
17. How reputable do you think most [health clubs/garages] are in general?
REFERENCES
Eisenberg, M. A. (1995). The limits of cognition and the limits of contract. Stanford Law Review, 47, 211±259.
Farnsworth, E. A. (1991). United States contract law. Farnsworth, E. A. (1990). Contracts. Boston: Little, Brown and Company. Felsenfeld, C., & Siegel, A. (1981). Writing contracts in plain English. St. Paul: West Publishing. Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1991). Social cognition. New York: McGraw-Hill. Gilmore, G. (1974). The death of contract. Columbus, Ohio: Ohio State University Press. Harrison, J. L. (1994). Class, personality, contract, and unconscionability. William & Mary Law
Review, 35, 445±501. Hasen, R. L. (1990). Efficiency under informational asymmetry: The effect of framing on legal rules.
University of California Los Angeles Law Review, 38, 391±438. Kuklin, B. (1988). On the knowing inclusion of unenforceable contract and lease terms. University of
Cincinnati Law Review, 56, 845±918. Macaulay, S. (1963). Non-contractual relations in business: A preliminary study. American Sociological
Review, 28, 55±67. Macaulay, S. (1985). An empirical view of contract. Wisconsin Law Review, 1985, 465±482. MacNeil, I. R. (1985). Relational contract: What we do and do not know. Wisconsin Law Review,
1985, 483±525. Masson, M. E. J., & Waldron, M. A. (1994). Comprehension of legal contracts by non-experts:
Effectiveness of plain language redrafting. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8, 67±85. Monahan, J. & Walker, L. (1993). Social science in law: Cases and materials (3rd ed.). Westbury, New
York: The Foundation Press. Morant, B. D. (1995). Contracts limiting liability: A paradox with tacit solutions. Tulane Law Review,
69, 715±779. Morrison, E. W. & Robinson, S. L. (in press). When employees feel betrayed: A model of how
psychological contract violation develops. Academy of Management Journal. Mueller, W. (1970). Residential tenants and their leases: An empirical study. Michigan Law Review,
69, 247±298. Robinson, S. L. (in press). Trust and breach of the psychological contract. Administrative Science
Quarterly. Robinson, S. L., & Rousseau, D. M. (1994). Violating the psychological contract: not the exception
but the norm. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 15, 245±259. Rousseau, D. M. (1989). Psychological and implied contracts in organizations. Employee
Responsibilities and Rights Journal, 2, 121±139. Rousseau, D. M., & Aquino, K. (1993). Fairness and implied contract obligations in job terminations:
the role of remedies, social accounts, and procedural justice. Human Performance, 6, 135±149. Rubin, E. L. (1995). The nonjudicial life of contract: Beyond the shadow of the law. Northwestern
University Law Review, 90, 107±131. Schmedemann, D. A., & McLean Parks, J. (1994). Contract formation and employee handbooks:
Legal, psychological, and empirical analyses. Wake Forest Law Review, 29, 647±718. Speidel, R. W. (1995). The shifting domain of contract. Northwestern University Law Review, 90,
254±266. Stratman, J. F. (1988). Contract disclaimers in ERISA summary plan documents: A deceptive
practice? Industrial Relations Law Journal, 10, 350±380. Vukowich, W. T. (1993). Lawyers and the standard form contract system: A model rule that should
have been. Georgetown Journal of Legal Ethics, 6, 799±859. White, J. J. (1988). Promise fulfilled and principle betrayed. Annual Survey of American Law, 1988,
7±42.
94 D. P. Stolle and A. J. Slain
Copyright of Behavioral Sciences & the Law is the property of John Wiley & Sons, Inc. and its content may not
be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder’s express written
permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.
-
TheAccumulationofStereotyoe-BasedSelf-fulfillingProphecies.pdf